Vai ai contenuti. | Spostati sulla navigazione | Spostati sulla ricerca | Vai al menu | Contatti | Accessibilità

| Crea un account

Charbonnier, Monique (2008) La comprensione della comunicazione non verbale nei bambini bilingui. [Tesi di dottorato]

Full text disponibile come:

[img]
Anteprima
Documento PDF
9Mb

Abstract (inglese)

The main objective of the present study was to highlight possible advantages determined by experience with two diverse linguist codes in the comprehension of non-verbal communication. This specifically, in those communicative acts that, to be understood, require: good meta-cognitive competence, meta-representational ability, inferential ability and the capability to attribute to this and to other cognitive states, and to forsee the behaviors on the basis of these inferences. The communicative acts investigated were: direct, indirect, deceitful and ironic actions. Two different groups were examined, one in Canada and one in Italy. The group in Canada consisted of 100 children; 50 French-English bilingual and 50 monolingual English children. The group in Italy consisted of 50 children; 25 French-Italian bilingual and 25 monolingual Italian children. Results of the present study indicate that the ability to speak two languages aids in the comprehension of non-verbal communicative acts. In particular, this advantage is evident in those communications that require comprehension of intentions in complex situations (deceitful and ironic actions)


Statistiche Download - Aggiungi a RefWorks
Tipo di EPrint:Tesi di dottorato
Relatore:Benelli, Beatrice
Dottorato (corsi e scuole):Ciclo 19 > Corsi per il 19simo ciclo > PSICOLOGIA DELLO SVILUPPO E DEI PROCESSI DI SOCIALIZZAZIONE
Data di deposito della tesi:2008
Anno di Pubblicazione:2008
Parole chiave (italiano / inglese):comunicazione non verbale, bilinguismo
Settori scientifico-disciplinari MIUR:Area 11 - Scienze storiche, filosofiche, pedagogiche e psicologiche > M-PSI/04 Psicologia dello sviluppo e psicologia dell'educazione
Struttura di riferimento:Dipartimenti > Dipartimento di Psicologia dello Sviluppo e della Socializzazione
Codice ID:290
Depositato il:12 Set 2008
Simple Metadata
Full Metadata
EndNote Format

Bibliografia

I riferimenti della bibliografia possono essere cercati con Cerca la citazione di AIRE, copiando il titolo dell'articolo (o del libro) e la rivista (se presente) nei campi appositi di "Cerca la Citazione di AIRE".
Le url contenute in alcuni riferimenti sono raggiungibili cliccando sul link alla fine della citazione (Vai!) e tramite Google (Ricerca con Google). Il risultato dipende dalla formattazione della citazione.

1. Alibali, M. W., Heath, D.C., & Myers, H.J. (2001). Effects of visibility between speakear and listener on gesture production: Some gestures are meant to be seen. Journal of Memory and Language, 44, 169-188. Cerca con Google

2. Alibali, M. W. & Kita, S. (2004). On the role of gesture in thinking and speaking: Prohibiting gesture alters children’s problem explanations. Manuscript under review. Cerca con Google

3. Alibali, M. W., Kita, S., & Young, A. J. (2000). Gesture and the process of speech production: we think, therefore we gesture. Language and Cognitive Processes, 15, 593-613. Cerca con Google

4. Alibali, M. W., Kita, S., Bigelow, L. J., Wolfman, C. M., & Klein, S. M. (2001). Gesture plays a role in thinking for speaking. In C. Cavé, I. Guaïtella & S. Santi (Eds.), oralité: et gesturalité: Interactions et comportements multimodaux dans la communication (pp. 407-410). Paris: L'Harmattan. Cerca con Google

5. Anolli L., Ciceri R. e Infantino M. G. (2000a). “Le voci dell’ironia: analisi dei profili acustici della comunicazione ironica”, Giornale Italiano di Psicologia, 4: 719-756, Cerca con Google

6. Anolli L., Ciceri R. e Infantino M. G. (2000b). “Irony as a game of implicitness: acoustic profiles of ironic communication”, Journal of Pscholinguistic Research, 29, 3, 275- 311. Cerca con Google

7. Astington, J.W., Olson, D.R. (1995). The cognitive revolution in children’s understanding of mind. Human Development, 38, 179-189. Cerca con Google

8. Astington, J.W. (2003), “Sometimes necessary, never sufficient: false belief understanding and social competence”, in Repacholi B. e Slaughter V. (a cura di), Individual differences in Theory of Mind. Implication for typical and atipical development, Blackwell Publisher, Oxford. Cerca con Google

9. Bachman, L., (1990,).Fundamental considerations in language testing, Oxford, Oxford University Press. Cerca con Google

10. Bara, Bruno G, (1999). Pragmatica cognitiva. I processi mentali della comunicazione. Bollati Boringhieri, Torino. Cerca con Google

11. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M. e Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 37-46. Cerca con Google

12. Baron-Cohen S. (1989). Perceptual role-taking and proto-declarative pointing in autism. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 7, 113-127. Cerca con Google

13. Baron-Cohen, S. (1991). Precursors to a theory of mind: understanding attention in others. In A. Whiten (a cura di), Natural theories of mind, Oxford: Basil Blackwell. Trad. it. in L. Camaioni, 2003. Cerca con Google

14. Baron-Cohen, S. (1993). From attention-goal psychology to belief-desidere psychology: the development of theory of mind and its dysfunction. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg e D.J. Cohen (a cura di). Understanding other minds:perspectives from autism (pp. 59-82). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Cerca con Google

15. Baron-Cohen, S. (1994). How to build a baby that can read minds: Cognitive mechanisms in mind reading. Cahiers de Psychologie Cognitive (Current psychology of Cognition), 13 (5), 1-40. Cerca con Google

16. Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A.M., Frith, U. (1995). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”?. Cognition, 4, 37-46. Cerca con Google

17. Baron-Cohen, S. (1995). Mindblindness. An essay on autism and theory of mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Trad. it. L’autismo e la lettura della mente. Roma: Astrolabio, 1997. Cerca con Google

18. Bartsch, K., Wellman, H.M. (1995). Children talk about the mind. New York: Oxford University Press. Cerca con Google

19. Battistelli, P. (2001). Teoria infantile della mente e pensiero metarappresentativo. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia, XXVIII, 3, 491-497. Cerca con Google

20. Beattie, G. & Shovelton, H. (1999). Mapping the range of information contained in the iconic hand gestures that accompany spontaneous speech. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 18, 438-462. Cerca con Google

21. Beeman, M. M., Davis, L. H., Carlisle, J. F., (1999). Learning to Write in Two Languages: A Study of Emerging Bilingualism. University Research Grant, Northwestern University. Cerca con Google

22. Bekkering, H., Wohlschlaeger, A., & Gattis, M. (2000). Imitation of gestures in children is goal-directed. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 1, 153–164. Cerca con Google

23. Benelli B., Belacchi C. La doga non è la moglie del doge. Un modello di sviluppo della competenza definitoria. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia. 4; 1999, pg. 703 – 740 Cerca con Google

24. Ben-Zeev, S. (1977). The influente of bilingualism on cognitive development. Child Development, pp: 1009/1018. Cerca con Google

25. Ben-Zeev, S. (1977). Mechanism by which childood bilingualism affect understanding of language and cognitive structures. In P. A. Hornby (a cura di) Bilingualism: Psychological, social and educational implications, New York Academic Press. Cerca con Google

26. Ben-Zeev, S. (1997). Mechanisms by which childhood bilingualism affects understanding of language and cognitive structures. In P. A. Hornby (ed.), Bilingualism: Psychological, social and educational implications, pp. 29-55. New York: Academic Press. Cerca con Google

27. Bialystok, E. &Ryan E.B. (1985). Toward a definition of metalinguistic skill, Merrill- Palmer Quarterly 31(3), pp. 229-251. Cerca con Google

28. Bialystok, E. (1988). Levels of bilingualism and levels of linguistic awareness. Developmentel Psychology, 24, 560-567. Cerca con Google

29. Bialystok, E. (1991). Language processing in bilingual children. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. Cerca con Google

30. Bialystok, E. (1992). Selective attention in cognitive processing: The bilingual edge. In R. J. Harris (ed.), Cognitive processing in bilinguals, pp. 501-513. New York: Elsevier Science Publishers. Cerca con Google

31. Bialystok, E. (1999). Cognitive complexity and attentional control in the bilingual mind. Child Development, 70, 636-644. Cerca con Google

32. Bialystok, E. & Codd, J. (1997). Cardinal limits: Evidente from language awareness and bilingualism for developing concepts of number, Cognitive Development, 12, 85- 106. Cerca con Google

33. Bialystok, E. & Majumder, S. (1998). The relationship between bilingualism and the development of cognitive processes in problem solving. Applied Psycholinguistics, 19, 69-85. Cerca con Google

34. Bucciarelli M., Colle L. & Bara B. G. (2003). How children comprehend speech acts and communicative gestures. Journal of Pragmatics 35, 207-241. Cerca con Google

35. Butterworth, B., & Hadar, U. (1989). Gesture, speech, and computational stages: A reply to McNeill. Psychological Review, 96, 168-174. Cerca con Google

36. Camaioni, L. (1993a). Lo sviluppo della teoria della mente e dei suoi precursori. Età Evolutiva, 45, 61-72. Cerca con Google

37. Camaioni, L. (1993b). The development of intentional communication. A re-analysis. In J. Nadel, L. Camaioni (Eds.), New perspectives in early comunicative development. London: Routledge, 82-96. Trad. it. In O. Liverta Sempio e A. Marchetti (a cura di), Il pensiero dell’altro: contesto, conoscenza e teorie della mente. Milano: Cortina, 1995, 319-333. Cerca con Google

38. Camaioni, L., Longobardi, E. (1997). Referenze a stati mentali nella produzione linguistica spontanea a 20 mesi. Età Evolutiva, 56, 16-25. Cerca con Google

39. Camaioni, L. (2001). Il contributo della teoria della mente alla comprensione dello sviluppo umano. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia, 3, 455-476. Cerca con Google

40. Camaioni, L. (a cura di) (2003). La Teoria della Mente. Origini, Sviluppo e Patologia. Roma-Bari: Laterza. Cerca con Google

41. Canale M., Swain M. (1980), Theoretical bases of communicative approaches to second language teaching and testing, Applied Linguistics 1, 1-47. Cerca con Google

42. Caponi B., Cornoldi C., Trevisan P., L’insegnamento metacognitivo, L’Educatore, 20, 1993, 28-33. Cerca con Google

43. Carey, S. (1985). Conceptual Change in Childhood. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Cerca con Google

44. Carpenter, M., Call, J., &Tomasello, M. (2005). Twelve-and18-month-old scopy actions in terms of goals. Developmental Science, 8(1), F13–F20. Cerca con Google

45. Caselli M. C., Maragna N., Pagliari Rampelli L., Volterra V. (1994). Linguaggio e sordità. Firenze, La Nuova Italia. Cerca con Google

46. Castelfranchi C., Parisi D. (1980). Linguaggio, conoscenze e scopi. Il Mulino, Bologna. Cerca con Google

47. Castelfranchi C., (1981). Scopi esterni in “Rassegna italiana di Sociologia”, XXII, I, pp. 329-81. Cerca con Google

48. Castelfranchi C., Poggi I., (1990). Blushing as a discourse: Was Darwin Wrong?, in R. Crozier (ed.), Shyness and Embarrassment. Perspectives from Social Psychology, Cambridge University Press, New York, pp. 230-51 Cerca con Google

49. Caviglia, G. (2003). Attaccamento e Psicopatologia. Roma: Carocci. Cerca con Google

50. Caviglia, G. (2005). Teoria della Mente, Attaccamento Disorganizzato, Psicopatologia. Roma: Carocci. Cerca con Google

51. Church, R.B., Ayman-Nolley, S.,& Estrade, J.(2004).The effects of gestural instruction on bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingual Education, in press. Cerca con Google

52. Church, R.B., & Goldin-Meadow, S.(1986).The mismatch between gesture and speech as anindex of transitional knowledge. Cognition, 23, 43–71. Cerca con Google

53. Clark H. H. e Gerrig R. J. (1984) On the pretense theory of irony, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113 (1): 121-26. Cerca con Google

54. Cohen, L.B. (1973). A two process model of infant visual attention, Merril Palmer Quarterly, 19, 157-180. Cerca con Google

55. Cohen, A.A. (1977). The communicative functions of hand illustrators. Journal of Communications, 27, 54-63. Cerca con Google

56. Cornoldi, C. (1995). Metacognizione e Apprendimento. Bologna: Il Mulino. Cerca con Google

57. Creusere M. A. (1997). The conceptual underpinnings of children’s understanding of ironic speech act, Poster presentato al biennal meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Washington, DC. Cerca con Google

58. Dews S. e Winner E. (1995). Muting the meaning: a social function of irony, Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 10(1): 3-19. Cerca con Google

59. Dews S., Kaplan J. e Winner E. (1995). Why not say it directly? The social functions of irony, Discorse Processes, 19: 347-367. Cerca con Google

60. Dews S., Winner E., Kaplan J., Rosenblatt E., Hunt M., Lim K., McGovern A., Qualter A. e Smarsh B. (1996). Children’s understanding of the meaning and functions of verbal irony, Child Development, 67: 3071-3085. Cerca con Google

61. Diane Gérin-Lajoie (2003) Parcours identitaires de jeunes francophones en milieu minoritarire, Prise de Parole. Cerca con Google

62. Dunn, J. (2000). Mind-reading, emotion understanding, and relationships. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24, 142-144 Cerca con Google

63. Ekman P., (1972). Universal and cultural differences in facial expression of emotion. In Cole J.K. (a cura di), Nebraska Symposium on Motivation 1971. University of Nebraska, Press, Lincoln. Cerca con Google

64. Ekman P., Friesen W. V. (1969). The Reportoire of Nonverbal Behavior: Categories, Origins, Usage and Coding, in Semiotica, I, I, pp. 49-98 Cerca con Google

65. Fantz R. L. (1966). Pettern discrimination and selective attention as determinants of perceptual development from birth. In Kidd L. e Rivoire G. ( a cura di), Perceptual development in children, International University Press, New York. Cerca con Google

66. Feldman, C.F. (1992). The new theory of theory of mind. Human Development, 35, 107-117. Cerca con Google

67. Flavell, J.H., Miller, P.H., Miller, A.S. (1977). Cognitive Development, (3rd Ed. 1993) US: Prentice-Hall. Trad. it. Psicologia dello Sviluppo Cognitivo, Bologna: Il Mulino, 1996. Cerca con Google

68. Flavell, J.H. (1979). Metacognition and Cognitive Monitoring : A New Area of Cognitive- Developmental Inquiry, American Psychologist, 34, 906-911. Cerca con Google

69. Flavell, J.H. (1999). Cognitive development: Children’s knowledge about the mind. Annual Review of Psychology, 50, 21-45. Cerca con Google

70. Flavell, J.H. (2000). Development of children’s knowledge about the mental world. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 24, 15-23. Cerca con Google

71. Flaves L.M.,& Perry,M.(2001).How many do you see? Theuse of non spoken representation sin first-grade mathematics lessons. Journal of Educational Psychology, 93, 330–345. Cerca con Google

72. Feyereisen P. e Seron X. (1982) “Non verbal communication an Aphasia: A Review: I Comprehension. II. Expression”. Brain and Language, 16, pp. 191-212, 213-236. Cerca con Google

73. Feyereisen P., e de Lanoy (1991) Gestures and Speech. Psychological Investigations. Cambridge, Cambridge University Press. Cerca con Google

74. Fodor, J.A. (1983). The modularity of mind. Cambridge, MA.: MIT Press. Cerca con Google

75. Fodor, J.A. (1992). A theory of the child’s theory o mind. Cognition, 44, 283-296. Cerca con Google

76. Fonagy, P., Target, M. (2001). Attaccamento e Funzione riflessiva. A cura di V. Lingiardi e M. Ammaniti. Milano: Raffaello Cortina. Cerca con Google

77. Fowler H.W. (1965), A dictionary of modern English usage (2 ed.), Oxford University Press, Oxford. Cerca con Google

78. Francis N. (1999) Bilinguism, writing and metalinguistic awarness: oral-literate interactions between first and second language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 20, pp. 553-561. Cerca con Google

79. Freedman N. (1972) The Analysis of Movement Behavior durino the Clinical Interview. In A. R. Siegman and B. Pope (Eds) Studies in Dyadic Communication. Elmsford, New York, Pergamon. Cerca con Google

80. Frye, D. (1991). The origins on intention in infancy. In D. Frye, C. Moore (Eds.), Children’s theories of mind: mental states and social understanding. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum. Cerca con Google

81. Frye, D., Moore, C. (Eds.) (1991). Children’s theories of mind: mental states and social understanding. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum. Cerca con Google

82. Genesee, F., Boivin, I. & Nicoladis, E. (1996). Talking with strangers: A study of bilingual children’s communicative competence. Applied Psycholinguistics, 17, 427- 442. Cerca con Google

83. Genesee, F., Nicoladis, E. & Paradis, J. (1995). Language differentation in early bilingual development. Journal of Child Language, 22, 611-631. Cerca con Google

84. Genesee, F., Tucker, G. R. & Lambert, W. E. (1975). Communication skills of bilingual children. Child Development, 46, 1010-1014. Cerca con Google

85. Giora R. (1995). On irony and negation, Discorse Processes, 19(2): 239-64. Cerca con Google

86. Gobbo, C. (2001). Cercando le basi dello sviluppo della Teoria della Mente. Giornale Italiano di Psicologia. XXVIII, 3, 507-516. Cerca con Google

87. Goldin-Meadow, S. & Butcher, C. (2003). Pointing toward two-word speech in young children. In S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet, (pp. 85-107). Mawhaw, N.J.: Erlbaum Cerca con Google

88. Goldin-Meadow, S., Kim, S., &Singer, M. (1999). What the teacher’shands tell the student’s mind about math. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 720–730. Cerca con Google

89. Goldin-Meadow, S. & Sandhofer, C. M. (1999). Gestures convey substantive information about a child's thoughts to ordinary listeners. Developmental Science, 2, 67-74. Cerca con Google

90. Goldin-Meadow, S., &Singer, M. A. (2003). From children’shands to adults’ears: Gesture’s role in teaching and learning. Developmental Psychology,39, 509–520. Cerca con Google

91. Goldman, A.I., (1993). The psychology of folk psychology. Behavioral and Brain Science, 16, 15-29. Cerca con Google

92. Gopnik, A. (2003). The theory theory as an alternative to innates hypothesis. In L. Antony & N. Horstein (Eds.) Chomsky and his critics (pp.238-254). New York: Basil Blackwell.Gopnik, A., Meltzoff, A.N. (1997). Words, thoughts, and theories. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Cerca con Google

93. Gopnik, A., Wellman, H.M. (1994). The „theory theory“. In L. Hirschfeld & S. Gelman (Eds.), Domain-Specificity in Cognition and Culture (pp. 257-293). New York: Cambridge university Press. Cerca con Google

94. Grice H.P. (1957). Meaning, Philosophical Review, 66: 377-88. Cerca con Google

95. Grice H.P. (1968). Utter’s Meaning, Sentence-Meaning, and World-Meaning, Foundations of Language, 4: 22-42. Cerca con Google

96. Grice H.P. (1969). Utter’s Meaning and Intention, Philosophical Review, 68: 147-77. Cerca con Google

97. Grice H.P. (1975). Logic and conversation, in Cole P. e Morgan J. L. (a cura di), Syntax and semantics, Volume III. Speech acts, Academic Press, New York pp. 41-58. Cerca con Google

98. Grice H.P. (1978). Further notes on logic and conversation, in Cole P. (a cura di), Syntax and semantics, Volume IX: Pragmatics, Academic Press, New York pp. 113- 27. Cerca con Google

99. Hakuta, K. (1987). Degree of bilingualism and cognitive ability in mainland Puerto Rican children. Child Development, 58, 1372-1388. Cerca con Google

100. Happé F.G.E. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: a test of relevance theory. Cognition, 48, 101-119. Cerca con Google

101. Happé F.G.E. (1995). “Understanding minds and metaphor: insights from the study of figurative language in autism”, Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 10(4): 275-295. Cerca con Google

102. Harris, P.L. (1992). From simulation to folk psychology: the case for development. Mind and Language, 7, 120-144. Cerca con Google

103. Harris, P.L. (1996). Desires, beliefs, and language. In P. Carruthers, P.K. Smith (Eds.). Theories of Theories of Mind (pp. 200-220). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Cerca con Google

104. Harris, P.L. (1996b). Comparing beliefs and desires. In P. Carruthers, P.K. Smith (Eds.). Theories of Theories of Mind. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Cerca con Google

105. Harris M. e Pexman P.M. (2003). Chidren’s perception of the social functions of verbal irony, Discorse Processes, 36,3: 147-165. Cerca con Google

106. Hornby, P.A. (Ed.) (1977). Bilingualism: Psychological, social, and educational implications. New York: Accademic Press. Cerca con Google

107. Hymes D.H., On communicative competence, Philadelphia, University of Pennsylvania Press (1971). Extracts available in: Brumfit, C.J. & Johnson K. (1979) (Eds.), The communicative approach to language teaching, pp. 5-26, Oxford, Oxford University Press. Cerca con Google

108. Iverson, J. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1998). Why people gesture when they speak. Nature, 396, p.228. Cerca con Google

109. Janoo-Worral A. D. (1972). Bilingualism and cognitive development. Child Development, 43, pp. 1390-1400. Cerca con Google

110. Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1988). The child is a theoretician, not an inductivist. Mind and Language, 3, 183-195. Cerca con Google

111. Keil, F.C. (1989). Concepts, Kinds, and Cognitive Development, Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Cerca con Google

112. Kelly,S.D.,Barr,D.J.,Church,R.B.,&Lynch,K.(1999).Offering a hand to pragmatic understanding: The role of speech and gesture in comprehension and memory. Journal of Memory & Language, 40, 577–592. Cerca con Google

113. Kendon A. (1995). Gestures as Illocutionary and Discorse Structure Markers in Southern Italian Conversation. Journal of Pragmatics, 23, pp. 247-279. Cerca con Google

114. Kendon A. (2004). Gesture. Visibile Action as Utterance. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. Cerca con Google

115. Kendon, A. (1994). Do gestures communicate? A review. Research on Language and Social Interaction, 27, 175-200. Cerca con Google

116. Kita S. (2000). How representational gestures help speaking. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 162-185). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Cerca con Google

117. Kita S. (ed.) (2003) Pointing. Erlbaum, Hillsdale. Cerca con Google

118. Krauss, R., Chen, Y., & Gottesman, R. (2000). Lexical gestures and lexical access: A process model. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture. Cambridge, (pp.261- 283). Cerca con Google

119. Lacroix J. M. & Rioux Y. (1978) La communication non-verbale chez les bilingues. Canad, J. Behav. Sciente, 10 (2). Cerca con Google

120. Leslie, A. M. (1987). Pretende and representation: the origins of “theory of mind”. Psychological Review, 94, 412-426. Cerca con Google

121. Leslie, A.M., Happé, F. (1989). Autism and ostensive communication: the relevance of metarepresentation. Development and Psychopathology, 1, 205-212. Cerca con Google

122. Leslie, A.M., Roth, D. (1993). What autism teaches us about metarepresentation. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Flusberg, D.J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Autism. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Cerca con Google

123. Leslie, A. M. (1994). ToMM, ToBy, and agency: Core architecture and domain specificity. In Hirschfeld & Gelman (eds.), pp. 119-148. Cerca con Google

124. Leslie, A.M. (2000). ‘Theory of mind’ as a mechanism of selective attention. In M. Gazzaniga (Ed.) The cognitive neurosciences, (pp. 1235-1247). Cabridge, MA: MIT Press. Cerca con Google

125. Lucariello J. (1997), “Dalla narrazione all’ironia. Lo sviluppo del ragionamento metarappresentativo”, in Smorti A., Il sé come testo: costruzione delle storie e sviluppo della persona, Giunti, Firenze. Cerca con Google

126. Marchetti, A. (1995). L’accesso alla mente: panorami e percorsi. In O. Liverta Sempio, A. Marchetti (a cura di), Il pensiero dell’altro. Milano: Cortina. Cerca con Google

127. Marchetti A. e Massaro D., “Quando piove e splende il sole. Ironia e stati mentali”, in Marchetti A. e Liverta Sempio O. (a cura di) (2004), Uno con molti. Cultura e costruzione dell’individualita’. Utet libreria, Torino. Cerca con Google

128. Marvin, R. S., Greenberg, M. T. e Mossler, D. G. (1976). The early development of conceptual perspective taking: distinguishing among multiple perspectives. Child Development, 47, 511-514. Cerca con Google

129. Mayberry, R. L., Jaques, J., & DeDe, G. (1998). What stuttering reveals about the developmental of the gesture-speech relationship. In J. Iverson & S. Golding- Meadow (Eds.), New direction for child development: the nature and functions of gesture in chlidren’s communication (Vol. 79, pp. 77-87). San Francisco: Jossey- Bass Cerca con Google

130. McNeill, D. (1985). So you think gestures are nonverbal. Psychological Review, 92, 350- 371. Cerca con Google

131. McNeill,D.(1992).Handandmind:What gestures reveal about thought.Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Cerca con Google

132. McNeill, D (1992). Hand and Mind. Chicago, University of Chicago Press. Cerca con Google

133. McNeill, D. & Duncan, S. (2000). Growth points in thinking-for-speaking. In D. McNeill, (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp.141-161). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Cerca con Google

134. Mehrabian A. (1956). Nonverbal Communication, Psychological Review, 63, 129-138. Cerca con Google

135. Meltzoff, A.N., Gopnik, A. (1993). The role of initation in understanding persons and developing a theory of mind. In S. Baron-Cohen, H. Tager-Fls berg e D.J. Cohen (Eds.), Understanding other minds. Oxford : Oxford University Press, 181-203. Trad. it. in L. Camaioni (a cura di), La teoria della mente. Origini, sviluppo e patologia. Bari : Laterza, 2003. Cerca con Google

136. Miceli M., Castelfranchi C. (1994). Le difese della mente, Carocci, Roma Cerca con Google

137. Moore, C. (1996). Theories of mind in infancy. British Journal of Developmental psychology, 14, 19-40 Cerca con Google

138. Muecke D.C. (1969). The compass of irony, Methuen and Co., London. Cerca con Google

139. Neill, S. (1991). Classroom nonverbal communication. London: Routledge. Cerca con Google

140. Paradis, J. and Genesee, F. (1996) Syntactic acquisition in bilingual children. Studies in Second Language Acquisition18, 1–25. Cerca con Google

141. Peggy J. Goestz (2003). The effects of bilingualism on theory of mind development. Cambridge, University Press. Cerca con Google

142. Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Poggi I. (2006) Le parole del corpo. Carocci. Cerca con Google

143. Poggi I., Pelachaud C. (2000a) Performative Facial Expression in Animated Faces, in J. Cassel, J. Sullivan, S. Prevost, E. Churchill (eds.), Embodied Conversational Agents, MIT Press, Cambridge (MA), pp. 155-88. Cerca con Google

144. Poulin-Dubois, D. e Shultz, T.R. (1988). The development of the understanding of human behavior: from agency to intentionality. In J.W. Astington, P.L. Harris e D.R. Olson (a cura di) (1988), Developing theories of mind (pp. 109-125). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Cerca con Google

145. Premack D. e Woodruff G. (1978), “Does the Chimpanzees have a Theory of Mind?”, The behavioural and brian sciences, I: 515-526. Cerca con Google

146. Rauscher, F. H., Krauss, R. M., & Chen, Y. (1996). Gesture, speech, and lexical access: The role of lexical movements in speech production Psychological Science, 7, 226- 231. Cerca con Google

147. Ricci Bitti P. E. e Zani B. (1983) La comunicazione come processo sociale. Bologna, Il Mulino. Cerca con Google

148. Rime B. e Schiaratura L. (1991) Gesture and Speech. In R. Feldman and B. Rime (Eds) Fundamentals of Nonverbal Behavior. New York, Cambridge University Press. Cerca con Google

149. Russel, J. (1995). « At two with nature»: Agency and the development of self-world dualism. In J. Bermudez, A. Marcel, N. Elian (Eds.), The Body and the Self, Cambridge MA: MIT Press. Cerca con Google

150. Segal, G. (1996). The modularity of theory of mind. In P. Carruhers e P.K. Smith (Eds.), Theories of theories of mind. Cambridge: Cambridge university Press. (pp. 141- 157). Cerca con Google

151. Semerari, A. (2000). Storia, teorie e tecniche della psicoterapia cognitiva. Roma-Bari: Laterza Cerca con Google

152. Shannon, C.E. e Weaver, W., (1949) The Mathematical Theory of Communication, University of Illinois Press, Urbana. Cerca con Google

153. Shatz, M. (1983). Communication. In J. Flavell e E.M. Markman (a cura di), The handbook of child psychology, Vol. 3: Cognitive development (pp. 841-885). New York: Wiley. Cerca con Google

154. Sperber, Dan & Deirdre Wilson (1986). Relevance: Communication and Cognition. Oxford: Blackwell. Cerca con Google

155. Sperber D., Wilson D. (1991) Irony and the use-mention distinction, in Davis S. (ed.) Prgmatics: A Reader, Oxford University Press, Oxfords, pp. 550-563. Cerca con Google

156. Sperber D., Wilson D. (1998) Irony and relevance. A reply to Drs. Seto, Hamamoto and Yamanashi, in Carton R., Uchida S. (a cura di), Relevance Theory: Applications and Implications, Benjaminm Amsterdam, pp. 283-293. Cerca con Google

157. Sperber, Dan & Wilson, Deirdre. (2002). Pragmatics, modularity and mind-reading. Mind & Language 17: 3-23. Special Issue on Pragmatics and Cognitive Science. Cerca con Google

158. Stich, S. (1983). From Folk Psychology to Cognitive Science, Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. Trad. it. Dalla Psicologia del Senso Comune alla Scienza Cognitiva. Bologna: Il Mulino, 1994. Cerca con Google

159. Sullivan, Kate, Winner, Ellen, Hopfield, Natalie (1995) How children tell a lie from a joke: The role of second-order mental state attributions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology 13, 191-204. Cerca con Google

160. Tirassa M. (1991) Fallimenti della comunicazione, in Ricerche di Psicologia, 3, pp. 59- 88. Cerca con Google

161. Thompson,L.A.,& Massaro,D.W.(1986).Evaluation an dintegration of speech and pointing gestures during referential understanding. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 42, 144–168. Cerca con Google

162. Thompson,L.A.,& Massaro,D.W.(1994).Children’s integration of speech and pointing gestures in comprehension. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 57, 327– 354. Cerca con Google

163. Tomasello, M. (1995). Joint attention as social cognition. In C. Moore, P. Dunham (Eds.), Joint attention: its origins and role in development, Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Cerca con Google

164. Utsumi A. (2000). Verbal irony as implicit display of ironic environment; Distinguishing ironic utterances from nonirony, Journal of Pragmactis, 32: 1777-806. Cerca con Google

165. Vygotskij, L.S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge: MIT Press. Trad. it. Pensiero e linguaggio, Roma-Bari Laterza, 1991. Cerca con Google

166. Vygotskij, L.S. (1973) tr.it. Pensiero e linguaggio. Ricerche psicologiche, Bari: Laterza, 2004, pp. 67-83. Cerca con Google

167. Wellman, H. M. e Bartsch, K. (1994). Before belief: Children’s early psychological theory. In C. Lewis e P. Mitchell (a cura di), Children’s early understanding of mind (pp. 331-354). Hove, Uk: Elbaum. Cerca con Google

168. Wellman, H.M., Gelman, S. (1997). Knowledge acquisition in foundational domains. In D. Kuhn & R. Siegler (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology (pp. 523-573). New York: Wiley. Cerca con Google

169. Wimmer, H.. e Perner J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition 13, 103-128. Cerca con Google

170. Winner E. e Leekam S. (1991). Distinguishing irony from deception: understanding the speaker’s second-order intention, British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9: 257-270. Cerca con Google

171. Yelland G. W., Pollard J., Mercuri A. (1993). The metalinguistic benefits of limited contact with a second language. Applied Psycholinguistics, pp: 14, 423-444. Cerca con Google

172. Zuanelli Sonino, E. (1981). La competenza comunicativa, Torino, Boringhieri. Cerca con Google

Download statistics

Solo per lo Staff dell Archivio: Modifica questo record